Samenvatting
Over the past decade, the field of organization studies has increasingly focused on
knowledge and knowledge processes in organizations. This thesis describes a study of
one of those processes, knowledge sharing, in the context of industrial research.
Though the attention for knowledge in organizations is rather new, technical
communication in industrial research has been subject of research for over the last
fifty years (e.g., Herner 1954; Allen 1977; Tushman 1978; Keller 1994). One major
lesson that can be learned from past research is that technical communication
contributes to research performance. In a study of 1311 researchers from industry and
government laboratories, Pelz and Andrews (1966) already found a significant
correlation between a researcher’s performance and his communication with
colleagues within and outside his group. Recently Anderson et al. (2001) established
again that colleagues are the most important source of information for researchers
(after the use of their own knowledge).
Although the importance of technical communication has been confirmed over and
over again, we do not know very much about the details of the contribution of
technical communication to industrial research practices. With a few exceptions
(Johnston and Gibbons 1975; Faulkner and Senker 1995), the content of technical
communication has not been systematically studied. Nevertheless, inspired by linear
communication models (e.g., Shannon and Weaver 1949), most studies in the field
have interpreted technical communication as the transfer of information or
knowledge (e.g., Gerstenfeld en Berger 1980; Kogut en Zander 1992; Moenaert en
Caeldries 1996). Furthermore, the way in which technical communication is
accomplished, is not systematically explored. Most of the things researchers might say
will be irrelevant for the work of others. Nevertheless researchers manage to
contribute to each others work. Several authors assume that this is because
communication is initiated by those in need of information or knowledge (e.g., Leckie
et al. 1996; Hansen 1999). Past research has found many factors that enable or
constrain knowledge sharing (e.g., Szulanski 1996; van der Bij et al. 2003), but these
studies did not yield insight in the ways in which communication is realized. The first
objective of the study reported in this dissertation was to gain insight in these issues:
in what ways does technical communication contribute to industrial research practices
and how is this accomplished.
This study had an important theoretical objective as well. Within the field of
organization studies two theoretical perspectives have offered an explanation for the
value of technical communication. These are the information processing approach
(Galbraith 1973; 1977; Tushman and Nadler 1978; Daft and Lengel 1986) and the
knowledge-based theory of the firm (Kogut and Zander 1992; 1996; Grant 1996a;
1996b; 2001). According to the information processing approach (IPA), organization
members face uncertainty and ambiguity, caused by task characteristics, task
interdependencies and the organizational environment. Furthermore, the IPA
assumes, in line with others, that technical communication consists of information
transfer, making it capable of reducing uncertainty and ambiguity. The central
hypothesis of the IPA is that higher performing organizations match their actual
information processing to the information processing needs associated with the
degrees of uncertainty and ambiguity they face. Thus, the core of the explanation for
the value of technical communication is that it is capable of reducing uncertainty or
ambiguity by transferring information.
Recently, the knowledge-based theory of the firm (KBT) has been developed. This
theory starts from the observation that a fundamental asymmetry exists between
knowledge development and knowledge application (Demsetz 1991). To develop a
thorough knowledge base, individuals have to specialize. However, to produce or
develop complex products, a broad range of knowledge is necessary. Therefore,
according to the KBT, the performance of an organization depends upon the
integration of the specialized knowledge of individuals, that is, upon the coordinated
application of this knowledge. The KBT, and the literature on knowledge
management, interprets technical communication first and foremost as knowledge
transfer. Knowledge transfer is interpreted as one among a number of possible
knowledge integration mechanisms. In this way the KBT offers an explanation of the
value of communication as well. However, the explanations of the IPA and the KBT
are not based on detailed investigations of the content and process of technical
communication. Van de Ven and Drazin (1985) note that the IPA uses information as
an abstract, latent, unmeasured variable. Grant (1996a; 1996b) calls for detailed
empirical research on knowledge sharing and knowledge integration to enhance his
theoretical analyses. Therefore, the second objective of this study was to use empirical
research results to reflect critically upon the assumptions and explanations of the IPA
and the KBT.
The objectives of this study called in the first place for exploratory, empirical research.
Past studies have emphasized that communication should be studied within the
context of the situated practices of group members (Lynch 1985; Orr 1990; Knorr-
Cetina 1995). For that reason, a choice was made to study communication among
mechanisms, of which 16 were found in the data. Examples of these origination
mechanisms are:
- ‘diffusing’: ego selecting existing information without an orientation at a specific
problem
- ‘pushing’: ego selecting existing information oriented at a problem of alter
- ‘pulled origination’: existing information is asked by alter
- ‘thinking along’: ego develops new content oriented at a problem of alter
- ‘self-suggesting’: ego develops new content oriented at one of his own problems
within an interaction
It should be noted that one interaction is often characterized by more than one
origination mechanism. This holds especially for interactive communication.
Quantitative analyses showed that these origination mechanisms are associated with
different moves and effects. For example, ‘diffusing’ is associated with the description
of own activities and own results, and reporting about others. Out of 26 cases (in a
total 227) in which this mechanism was found, 23 yielded an indirect contribution and
only 5 a direct contribution. Conversely, ‘thinking along’ is predominantly associated
with moves like giving arguments, suggesting experiments and technical solutions,
agreeing, rejecting, questioning and concluding. Thinking along yielded a direct
contribution in 26 out of 37 cases.
The concept of origination mechanisms is a contribution to the existing literature.
Only fragments of it have been acknowledged before, such as the distinction between
‘pushing’ and ‘pulling’ (e.g., Langrish et al. 1972; Schulz 2001). Furthermore, it
enables the identification of biases in the existing literature. For example, most
literature on information transfer assumes that this information is always existing
before communication, waiting to be transferred. Another bias in the existing
literature is that it assumes that information transfer and knowledge transfer are
initiated by information searching individuals.
A general finding with regard to origination mechanisms is that those origination
mechanisms that are oriented at a problem of alter or a shared problem are most
likely to yield direct contributions (in 82 out of 112 cases). Nevertheless, direct
contributions are also produced via originations that are not oriented at such a
problem and by self-suggesting: thinking up new ideas with regard to one’s own
problem in interaction. The effectiveness of origination mechanisms oriented at a
shared problem or a problem of alter depends upon the application and development
of knowledge. For example, asking a targeted question is enabled by knowing what
knowledge one is lacking and by knowing who has that knowledge. For thinking along
that is not necessary. But thinking along requires that ego knows about alter’s
problem and has relevant background knowledge. Each of the origination
mechanisms has its conditions for effective employment. Therefore it is not wise to
limit communication to one or a few mechanisms.
These empirical results were used to reflect upon the theoretical explanations of the
value of technical communication offered by the IPA and the KBT. With regard to the
IPA, the following conclusions were drawn. First, a reflection upon the effects of
technical communication showed that technical communication does not always
reduce uncertainty or ambiguity, as is assumed by the IPA. Communication also leads
to new questions and problems, to vanishing certainties and to conflicting
interpretations. The increase of uncertainty or ambiguity may be valuable effects of
technical communication in research as well. The assumption that technical
communication can be interpreted as information transfer is questionable too. Several
interpretations of ‘information’ were discussed (e.g., Shannon and Weaver 1949;
Dretske 1981). For example, if information is interpreted as a capacity to reduce
uncertainty or to increase knowledge (like Galbraith (1973) and Tushman and Nadler
(1978) assume), the assumption that useful technical communication can be
interpreted as information transfer is false. Both conclusions undermine the core of
the explanation of the value of communication by the IPA.
The KBT interprets technical communication as knowledge transfer, and knowledge
transfer as a knowledge integration mechanism. According to this perspective, the
integration of knowledge determines the performance of firms. However, a reflection
upon moves and effects showed that useful technical communication cannot always
be interpreted as knowledge transfer. Some instances of technical communication are
better described as ‘knowledge conducive communication’. Moreover, in contrast with
existing views, knowledge application and knowledge generation play a central role in
technical communication as well. But, in fact, this strengthens the explanation of the
KBT. The analysis of knowledge processes occurring in technical communication
shows other ways of integrating knowledge. For example, in thinking along ego
applies his knowledge to a problem of alter. In one of the studied episodes, an expert
in fracture mechanics applied his knowledge to determine the cause of a broken disc,
showed to him by a colleague. The expert communicated his conclusion but did not
communicate all knowledge on fracture mechanics that he used. This form of
knowledge integration, temporarily applying one’s knowledge to somebody else’s
problem, is more efficient than knowledge transfer, since not all knowledge that gets
integrated needs to be transferred. Thus, a number of origination mechanisms that
are effective in yielding direct contributions, including thinking along, are efficient as
well. Further, the discovery of this knowledge integration mechanism shows the
incompleteness of the taxonomy of mechanisms developed by Grant (1996b; 1997).
Insight in the effectiveness and efficiency of the origination mechanisms in which ego
selects or develops with an eye on a problem of alter or a shared problem was
furthered by interpreting research findings in terms of distributed cognition
(Hutchins 1995). In these origination mechanisms, including thinking along, a
particular distribution of cognitive labor is realized. The temporary distribution of the
cognitive labor over more people, for example during an interactive presentation,
improves creativity and reliability (probably in an efficient way). Finally this study
indicated that the value of this form of integrated cognitive work is not limited to
cases in which a need for task integration exists. That suggests that the focus on the
integration of knowledge and cognition is a valuable addition to the traditional
orientation on the integration of tasks and not just a semantical shift.
The research results provide a new explanation for the specific value of personal
communication. Existing explanations include that some information cannot be
found in published sources or databases (Holland et al. 1976), that personal
communication is a richer communication medium (Daft and Lengel 1986) and that
personal communication enables the transfer of tacit knowledge (Nonaka 1994). This
study suggests that personal communication enables those origination mechanisms
in which content is selected or developed by ego with an orientation at a problem of
alter or a shared problem. Enabled by the application of knowledge, these origination
mechanisms yield effective and efficient contributions to research practices. Finally, it
can be concluded that technical communication is a heterogeneous, active and
integral part of industrial research practices. These characteristics are insufficiently
captured by interpretations of technical communication as information transfer or
knowledge transfer.
Originele taal-2 | Engels |
---|---|
Kwalificatie | Doctor in de Filosofie |
Toekennende instantie |
|
Begeleider(s)/adviseur |
|
Datum van toekenning | 7 feb. 2003 |
Plaats van publicatie | Eindhoven |
Uitgever | |
Gedrukte ISBN's | 90-386-1967-7 |
DOI's | |
Status | Gepubliceerd - 2003 |